Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscripts Submit a manuscript HHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
Curr Opin Behav Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC4787559
NIHMSID: NIHMS762651
PMID: 26977424

Self-medication with sucrose

Abstract

For many individuals, stress promotes the consumption of sweet, high-sugar foods relative to healthier alternatives. Daily life stressors stimulate the overeating of highly-palatable foods through multiple mechanisms, including altered glucocorticoid, relaxin-3, ghrelin and serotonin signaling in brain. In turn, a history of consuming high-sugar foods attenuates the psychological (anxiety and depressed mood) and physiological (HPA axis) effects of stress. Together the metabolic and hedonic properties of sucrose contribute to its stress relief, possibly via actions in both the periphery (e.g., glucocorticoid receptor signaling in adipose tissue) and in the brain (e.g., plasticity in brain reward regions). Emerging work continues to reveal the bidirectional mechanisms that underlie the use of high-sugar foods as ‘self-medication’ for stress relief.

Introduction

Stress is typically defined as a real or perceived threat to homeostasis or well-being, and can be either physical (e.g., illness, injury) or psychological (e.g., financial insecurity, overscheduled time demands, troubled interpersonal relationships) in nature (reviewed in [1,2]). Stress has clear effects on mood and behavior, including increased anxiety and/or depressed mood. In addition, stress-regulatory brain circuits are activated during stress and orchestrate physiological responses that minimize homeostatic disruption and promote survival. Activation of pre-autonomic brain regions rapidly increases sympathetic nervous system (SNS) tone. This ‘fight or flight’ response acts to mobilize and distribute stored energy. In addition, hypophysiotropic neurons in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN) are activated during stress, resulting in the stimulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis. More specifically, activated PVN neurons release corticotropin releasing hormone (CRH) and other releasing hormones into the portal circulation, which then act on the anterior pituitary to evoke the secretion of adrenocorticotropin hormone (ACTH) into systemic circulation. ACTH stimulates the production and release of glucocorticoid hormones (e.g., cortisol in people and corticosterone in rodents) from the adrenal cortex. Circulating glucocorticoids then act on receptors (glucocorticoid receptor (GR) and mineralocorticoid receptor (MR)) throughout brain and body to promote and sustain the mobilization of stored energy, while also limiting further HPA axis activation via negative feedback in brain and pituitary. In this manner, coordinated activation of the SNS and HPA axis during an acute stress helps to maintain homeostasis and promote survival.

Excessive, repeated or chronic activation of the HPA axis and SNS has a number of negative consequences. For instance, excessive glucocorticoid exposure is associated with cognitive decline and diminished bone density, while persistent elevations in SNS drive are linked with cardiovascular dysfunction (reviewed in [2]). This implies that the magnitude of HPA axis and SNS responses should be ‘tuned’ to the particular needs of the individual. In addition, mounting evidence suggests that an individual's metabolic status and diet are important factors that impact the magnitude of physiological stress responses (reviewed in [2]). As a consequence, individuals may develop dietary and metabolic strategies to curb excessive activation of these stress systems. For example, people may increase their consumption of tasty, highly-palatable foods during stress because these so-called ‘comfort’ foods blunt the psychological and physiological impact of stress [3]. This review summarizes recent research centered on the idea that some individuals eat tasty, sugar-laden foods as ‘self-medication’ for stress.

Stress alters feeding behavior

The impact of stress on overall food intake varies considerably between individuals, with ∼ 35-70% of people reporting increased, and ∼ 25-40% reporting decreased, total caloric intake during stress [4-7]. Several factors have been identified that likely contribute to this variability in stress effects, such as the type and intensity of the stressor, as well as the individual's prior experience with food. Imminent, intense, direct physical threats are linked primarily with reduced food intake in both people [8,9] and rodents [10-12], whereas more mild “daily life” and social stressors may predispose for overeating in people [13-17], non-human primates [18] and rodents [19]. Moreover, a history or prior food restriction and being a ‘restrained’ or ‘emotional’ eater increases risk for stress-induced overeating in humans [5,16,17] and rodents [20].

In addition to impacting total food intake, stress also affects the types of food that are eaten. Stress generally promotes the consumption of highly-palatable, calorically-dense foods relative to less tasty, more nutritious foods [5,13,14,21-26], even among people who decrease their total caloric intake [5]. This is particularly true for high-sugar foods, with ∼ 60-70% of people reporting that they eat more sweet foods like candy, chocolate, cakes and cookies during times of high stress [5,7,25]. Likewise, the consumption of cake, chocolate and/or cookies approximately doubles during a laboratory-induced stressor [7], particularly for emotional eaters [22]. Consistent with this idea, chronic stress exposure to rodents typically reduces intake of their normal, nutritionally-balanced chow [10,27-29]. However, when highly-palatable foods like sucrose drink are also freely available, the intake of sucrose is preserved during stress [10,27-29], resulting in a greater proportion of daily calories from sugar. Furthermore, the anorectic effects of stress on chow intake is prevented in rats with a history of intermittent, but not continuous, access to sucrose, thus maintaining overall caloric intake during stress [29].

The mechanisms by which stress promotes the intake of sweet foods are not clear, but likely involve increasing the motivation or ‘wanting’ of these highly-palatable foods. Stress increases activation of brain reward-related structures by palatable foods [21,30], and people report that induction of a depressed, stress-like mood increases cravings for chocolate [31]. Moreover, both people and rats are willing to do more work to obtain sucrose, chocolate and other desserts during stress [31-33]. Notably, this increased motivation for high sugar foods can occur despite stress-induced anhedonia, in which the ‘liking’ of mildly sweet foods (such as dilute saccharin or sucrose drink) is reduced [10,11,34,35]. Stress-evoked glucocorticoids likely contribute to the increased motivation to eat during stress; glucocorticoids can act in brain to increase total food intake, as well as preference for sweet foods and other carbohydrates [36-40]. Recent evidence indicates that brain relaxin-3 also promotes sucrose intake following stress. Stress can increase the mRNA expression of relaxin-3 and its receptor in brain [41-43], and relaxin-3 signaling can enhance food intake, including the consumption of high-sugar foods [44-46]. Moreover, pharmacological antagonism of relaxin-3 receptor signaling in brain prevents stress-induced increases in sucrose consumption in binge-like eating prone rats (i.e., the subset of female rats that escalate their intermittent sucrose intake when given repeated footshock stress) [42]. Endocannabinoid signaling may also stimulate stress-eating, as loss of cannabinoid receptor 1 on serotonergic neurons blunts the increased sucrose preference that occurs following chronic social defeat stress in mice [47]. Lastly, the gut hormone ghrelin is induced by stress and may act in brain to promote palatable food intake during stress (reviewed in [48]).

It should also be noted that while many people increase sweet food intake during stress, not all do. Recent work suggests that some of these individual differences in stress-eating are related to genetic differences in serotonin neurotransmission. Following a laboratory stressor, people generally choose to eat sweet high-fat snacks more than savory high-fat snacks; however, this stress-induced preference for sweet fatty foods occurs primarily for people who are homozygous for a short-allele (S/S; as opposed to the long-allele, L/L) polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene [49]. Likewise, while both S/S and L/L subjects report greater appetite for sweet snacks following an academic exam stress, this occurs to a larger extent for the S/S subjects [50]. Collectively, this suggests that altered serotonin signaling may predispose for overeating high-sugar foods in response to stress. Consumption of these sweet foods may then act to confer both psychological and physiological stress relief, as described below.

Stress relief by dietary sucrose

When people who overeat tasty foods during stress are asked why they do so, 53% reply that the primary reason is because these foods make them feel better (i.e., more relaxed or comforted) [7]. Consistent with this idea, people often experience greater happiness, improved mood, and less perceived stress after eating sweet foods [51-53]. These psychological improvements are also accompanied by an attenuate HPA axis response to stress [54-56], particularly for stress-prone subjects [57]. Moreover, when rodents are given the option to eat highly-palatable foods like sucrose, their responses to stress are similarly blunted – these effects include both reduced HPA axis tone and diminished behavioral anxiety [10,27,29,58-64], indicating a capacity for both psychological and physiological stress relief.

Recent research has focused on understanding how high-sugar foods reduce stress, and has revealed that there are likely multiple mechanisms underlying these effects. Much of this work was borne from the initial observation that unlimited consumption of 30% sucrose drink, but not the artificial sweetener saccharin, normalizes the elevated CRH mRNA expression in the PVN that otherwise occurs following adrenalectomy (due to the loss of glucocorticoid negative feedback) [65]. These results indicate that HPA axis activity can be profoundly inhibited by the metabolic properties of sucrose (e.g., macronutrients, calories). Consistent with this idea, when adrenal-intact rats are given free access to 30% sucrose drink, they derive large portions of their daily calories from sucrose and have blunted HPA axis responses [59,61,64,66]. Furthermore, the expression of CRH mRNA is inversely related to the size of the mesenteric fat depot in sucrose-fed, adrenalectomized rats, leading to speculation that the visceral fat itself may limit the HPA axis through an unknown means [3,67]. New research now provides some evidence to support this possibility. Transgenic mice with reduced GR expression in adipose tissue have increased CRH mRNA expression in the PVN, as well as an elevated plasma corticosterone response to stress, indicating that GR-signaling in adipose tissue can exert an inhibitory influence on central HPA axis tone [68]. Although it is not yet clear whether this type of GR–adipose signal contributes to HPA inhibition by dietary sucrose.

Importantly, at least some of the stress-relieving properties of sucrose appear to depend on its pleasurable and rewarding properties, particularly when it is offered in limited amounts. When rats with free access to chow and water are given additional access to a small amount (4 ml) of 30% sucrose drink twice daily for 2 weeks, both anxiety-related behaviors and the HPA axis response to restraint stress are attenuated relative to water-controls [10,58]. This limited sucrose intake (LSI) paradigm does not alter total body weight nor percent body weight since rats reduce their chow intake slightly to compensate for the calories provided by the sucrose drink (∼9 calories/day, or ∼10% of total daily caloric intake) [10,58]. When the artificial sweetener saccharin (0.1%) is offered in this same paradigm, it reduces the HPA axis response similar to sucrose, and when the sucrose drink is instead administered by twice-daily gavage it no longer attenuates the HPA response to restraint [58]. Furthermore, when male rats are given limited intermittent access to another type of naturally rewarding behavior (sexual activity) the HPA axis response to stress is also diminished [58]. Collectively these results suggest that the blunted HPA reactivity that occurs following LSI does not depend upon the macronutrient and/or caloric properties of sucrose, but rather is primarily mediated by its hedonic and/or rewarding properties.

Consistent with this idea, neuronal activity in brain reward pathway appears to be critical for LSI stress relief. Sucrose intake activates several reward-regulatory brain sites, and over the long-term induces synaptic plasticity in these regions [58,69-71]. LSI also attenuates stress-induced cFos mRNA induction in the basolateral amygdala (BLA) – a brain region that is important for both reward and stress regulation [10]. Neuronal activity in the BLA is required for LSI stress relief since excitotoxic lesion of the BLA prior to LSI prevents the blunted HPA reactivity despite equivalent sucrose intake [58]. Finally, in order to more broadly identify the neuronal network by which LSI blunts HPA axis activity, statistical modeling was performed on a large dataset that measured multiple synaptic plasticity proteins in several reward- and stress-regulatory brain sites following LSI. The results implicated two concurrent neuronal pathways underlying the stress relief – reduced activity of a stress-excitatory pathway between the BLA and medial amygdala, as well as potentiation of a stress-inhibitory pathway from the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, which together result in less stress-excitatory drive to downstream effector sites like the PVN [69]. Future work can now empirically test these predictions to uncover the neuronal mechanisms by which the hedonic and rewarding properties of sucrose contribute to its stress-relieving effects. Further work will also be needed to determine whether the mechanisms of stress relief vary across particular conditions. For example, reward-based mechanisms likely predominate when the amount of sucrose consumed is relatively small, whereas the metabolism-based mechanisms may play a more prominent role as sucrose intake escalates.

Summary and perspectives

Stress causes many individuals to increase their consumption of sweet, high-sugar foods, in part because these foods provide stress relief. However, over the long term this coping strategy can promote obesity and metabolic dysfunction. Furthermore, the repeated consumption of high-sugar foods during stress may develop into a habit, and stress further engrains habitual behaviors [72] – making it increasingly difficult to stop the pattern of stress-eating. Additional insight into the mechanisms underlying sucrose ‘self-medication’ is needed, particularly since modern life combines easy access to high-sugar foods with highly-stressful lives.

Stress promotes the choice of sweet, high-sugar foods relative to less-tasty alternatives for many individuals, particularly when typical daily-life stressors (like work, school, and interpersonal relationships) are experienced. Stress likely stimulates the overeating of highly-palatable foods through multiple mechanisms, including altered glucocorticoid, relaxin-3, ghrelin and serotonin signaling in brain. In turn, a history of consuming high-sugar foods attenuates the physiological (e.g., HPA axis activation) and psychological (e.g., anxiety, negative mood) impact of stress. Both the metabolic and hedonic properties of sucrose contribute to its stress relief via a combination of effects in the periphery (e.g., GR-signaling in adipose tissue) and brain (e.g., plasticity in brain reward regions). Future work will undoubtedly reveal additional mechanistic insights into the bidirectional relationship between stress and high-sugar foods.

Highlights

  • For many individuals, stress promotes the consumption of sweet, high-sugar foods

  • Mediators of stress-eating include altered glucocorticoid and serotonin signaling

  • Sweet foods can also reduce the psychological and physiological impact of stress

  • Stress responses are blunted by both the metabolic and hedonic properties of sugar

  • This suggests that some people overeat high-sugar foods to relieve stress

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health (United States) grant R01 DK091425. The author thanks Dr. Karen Ryan (University of California at Davis) for her helpful comments on the manuscript.

Footnotes

The author has no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

1. Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP. Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:397–409. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
2. Ulrich-Lai YM, Ryan KK. Neuroendocrine circuits governing energy balance and stress regulation: functional overlap and therapeutic implications. Cell Metab. 2014;19:910–925. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
3. Dallman MF, Pecoraro N, Akana SF, La Fleur SE, Gomez F, Houshyar H, Bell ME, Bhatnagar S, Laugero KD, Manalo S. Chronic stress and obesity: a new view of “comfort food” Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:11696–11701. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
4. Epel E, Jimenez S, Brownell K, Stroud L, Stoney C, Niaura R. Are stress eaters at risk for the metabolic syndrome? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1032:208–210. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
5. Oliver G, Wardle J. Perceived effects of stress on food choice. Physiol Behav. 1999;66:511–515. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
6. Weinstein SE, Shide DJ, Rolls BJ. Changes in food intake in response to stress in men and women: psychological factors. Appetite. 1997;28:7–18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
7. Zellner DA, Loaiza S, Gonzalez Z, Pita J, Morales J, Pecora D, Wolf A. Food selection changes under stress. Physiol Behav. 2006;87:789–793. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
8. Bellisle F, Louis-Sylvestre J, Linet N, Rocaboy B, Dalle B, Cheneau F, L'Hinoret D, Guyot L. Anxiety and food intake in men. Psychosom Med. 1990;52:452–457. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
9. Popper R, Smits G, Meiselman HL, Hirsch E. Eating in combat: a survey of U.S. Marines. Mil Med. 1989;154:619–623. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
10. Ulrich-Lai YM, Ostrander MM, Thomas IM, Packard BA, Furay AR, Dolgas CM, Van Hooren DC, Figueiredo HF, Mueller NK, Choi DC, et al. Daily limited access to sweetened drink attenuates hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis stress responses. Endocrinology. 2007;148:1823–1834. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
11. Willner P, Moreau JL, Nielsen CK, Papp M, Sluzewska A. Decreased hedonic responsiveness following chronic mild stress is not secondary to loss of body weight. Physiol Behav. 1996;60:129–134. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
12. Reppucci CJ, Kuthyar M, Petrovich GD. Contextual fear cues inhibit eating in food-deprived male and female rats. Appetite. 2013;69:186–195. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
13. Epel E, Lapidus R, McEwen B, Brownell K. Stress may add bite to appetite in women: a laboratory study of stress-induced cortisol and eating behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2001;26:37–49. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
14. McCann BS, Warnick GR, Knopp RH. Changes in plasma lipids and dietary intake accompanying shifts in perceived workload and stress. Psychosom Med. 1990;52:97–108. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
15. Pollard TM, Steptoe A, Canaan L, Davies GJ, Wardle J. Effects of academic examination stress on eating behavior and blood lipid levels. Int J Behav Med. 1995;2:299–320. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
16. Rutters F, Nieuwenhuizen AG, Lemmens SG, Born JM, Westerterp-Plantenga MS. Acute stress-related changes in eating in the absence of hunger. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2009;17:72–77. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
17. Wardle J, Steptoe A, Oliver G, Lipsey Z. Stress, dietary restraint and food intake. J Psychosom Res. 2000;48:195–202. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
18. Michopoulos V, Toufexis D, Wilson ME. Social stress interacts with diet history to promote emotional feeding in females. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:1479–1490. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
19. Bartolomucci A, Cabassi A, Govoni P, Ceresini G, Cero C, Berra D, Dadomo H, Franceschini P, Dell'Omo G, Parmigiani S, et al. Metabolic consequences and vulnerability to diet-induced obesity in male mice under chronic social stress. PLoS One. 2009;4:e4331. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
20. Hagan MM, Wauford PK, Chandler PC, Jarrett LA, Rybak RJ, Blackburn K. A new animal model of binge eating: key synergistic role of past caloric restriction and stress. Physiol Behav. 2002;77:45–54. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
21. Tryon MS, Carter CS, Decant R, Laugero KD. Chronic stress exposure may affect the brain's response to high calorie food cues and predispose to obesogenic eating habits. Physiol Behav. 2013;120:233–242. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
22. Oliver G, Wardle J, Gibson EL. Stress and food choice: a laboratory study. Psychosom Med. 2000;62:853–865. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
23. Groesz LM, McCoy S, Carl J, Saslow L, Stewart J, Adler N, Laraia B, Epel E. What is eating you? Stress and the drive to eat. Appetite. 2012;58:717–721. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
24. Laugero KD, Falcon LM, Tucker KL. Relationship between perceived stress and dietary and activity patterns in older adults participating in the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study. Appetite. 2011;56:194–204. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
25. Kim Y, Yang HY, Kim AJ, Lim Y. Academic stress levels were positively associated with sweet food consumption among Korean high-school students. Nutrition. 2013;29:213–218. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
26. Cartwright M, Wardle J, Steggles N, Simon AE, Croker H, Jarvis MJ. Stress and dietary practices in adolescents. Health Psychol. 2003;22:362–369. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
27. Pecoraro N, Reyes F, Gomez F, Bhargava A, Dallman MF. Chronic stress promotes palatable feeding, which reduces signs of stress: feedforward and feedback effects of chronic stress. Endocrinology. 2004;145:3754–3762. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
28. Packard AE, Ghosal S, Herman JP, Woods SC, Ulrich-Lai YM. Chronic variable stress improves glucose tolerance in rats with sucrose-induced prediabetes. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;47:178–188. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
29. Martin J, Timofeeva E. Intermittent access to sucrose increases sucrose-licking activity and attenuates restraint stress-induced activation of the lateral septum. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2010;298:R1383–1398. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
**30. Rudenga KJ, Sinha R, Small DM. Acute stress potentiates brain response to milkshake as a function of body weight and chronic stress. Int J Obes (Lond) 2013;37:309–316. This manuscript shows that in overweight women, acute stress increases the response of the right amygdala (fMRI) to consumption of chocolate milkshake, and that this response is associated with resting plasma cortisol levels. This work demonstrates that acute stress can alter the brain response to a sweet, high-sugar food, and further suggests that this is related to an individual's level of chronic stress. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
31. Willner P, Benton D, Brown E, Cheeta S, Davies G, Morgan J, Morgan M. “Depression” increases “craving” for sweet rewards in animal and human models of depression and craving. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1998;136:272–283. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
32. Lemmens SG, Rutters F, Born JM, Westerterp-Plantenga MS. Stress augments food ‘wanting’ and energy intake in visceral overweight subjects in the absence of hunger. Physiol Behav. 2011;103:157–163. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
**33. Riga D, Theijs JT, De Vries TJ, Smit AB, Spijker S. Social defeat-induced anhedonia: effects on operant sucrose-seeking behavior. Front Behav Neurosci. 2015;9:195. This paper explores the effects of a chronic stress paradigm on multiple aspects of sucrose self-adminstration in rats. The work demonstrates that a history of chronic stress increases motivational drive for sucrose, delays sucrose withdrawal, and promotes reinstatement of sucrose self-administration. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
34. Muscat R, Willner P. Suppression of sucrose drinking by chronic mild unpredictable stress: a methodological analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1992;16:507–517. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
35. Willner P, Healy S. Decreased hedonic responsiveness during a brief depressive mood swing. J Affect Disord. 1994;32:13–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
36. Green PK, Wilkinson CW, Woods SC. Intraventricular corticosterone increases the rate of body weight gain in underweight adrenalectomized rats. Endocrinology. 1992;130:269–275. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
37. Tataranni PA, Larson DE, Snitker S, Young JB, Flatt JP, Ravussin E. Effects of glucocorticoids on energy metabolism and food intake in humans. Am J Physiol. 1996;271:E317–325. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
38. Tempel DL, McEwen BS, Leibowitz SF. Effects of adrenal steroid agonists on food intake and macronutrient selection. Physiol Behav. 1992;52:1161–1166. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
39. Zakrzewska KE, Cusin I, Stricker-Krongrad A, Boss O, Ricquier D, Jeanrenaud B, Rohner-Jeanrenaud F. Induction of obesity and hyperleptinemia by central glucocorticoid infusion in the rat. Diabetes. 1999;48:365–370. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
40. Bhatnagar S, Bell ME, Liang J, Soriano L, Nagy TR, Dallman MF. Corticosterone facilitates saccharin intake in adrenalectomized rats: does corticosterone increase stimulus salience? J Neuroendocrinol. 2000;12:453–460. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
41. Tanaka M, Iijima N, Miyamoto Y, Fukusumi S, Itoh Y, Ozawa H, Ibata Y. Neurons expressing relaxin 3/INSL 7 in the nucleus incertus respond to stress. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;21:1659–1670. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
*42. Calvez J, de Avila C, Matte LO, Guevremont G, Gundlach AL, Timofeeva E. Role of relaxin-3/RXFP3 system in stress-induced binge-like eating in female rats. Neuropharmacology. 2016;102:207–215. This paper identifies relaxin-3 as a mechanism by which stress promotes the intake of high-sugar foods in rodents that are inherently prone to stress-eating. The authors show that pharmacological antagonism of the relaxin-3 receptor (RXFP3) in brain prevents stress-induced increases in sucrose intake in ‘binge-like eating prone’ female rats (i.e., the subset of rats that escalate their sucrose consumption when given both unpredictable, intermittent (1 hour) access to 10% sucrose drink and repeated footshock stress) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
43. Banerjee A, Shen PJ, Ma S, Bathgate RA, Gundlach AL. Swim stress excitation of nucleus incertus and rapid induction of relaxin-3 expression via CRF1 activation. Neuropharmacology. 2010;58:145–155. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
44. Walker AW, Smith CM, Gundlach AL, Lawrence AJ. Relaxin-3 receptor (Rxfp3) gene deletion reduces operant sucrose- but not alcohol-responding in mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2015;14:625–634. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
45. Smith CM, Chua BE, Zhang C, Walker AW, Haidar M, Hawkes D, Shabanpoor F, Hossain MA, Wade JD, Rosengren KJ, et al. Central injection of relaxin-3 receptor (RXFP3) antagonist peptides reduces motivated food seeking and consumption in C57BL/6J mice. Behav Brain Res. 2014;268:117–126. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
46. Ganella DE, Callander GE, Ma S, Bye CR, Gundlach AL, Bathgate RA. Modulation of feeding by chronic rAAV expression of a relaxin-3 peptide agonist in rat hypothalamus. Gene Ther. 2013;20:703–716. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
47. Dubreucq S, Matias I, Cardinal P, Haring M, Lutz B, Marsicano G, Chaouloff F. Genetic dissection of the role of cannabinoid type-1 receptors in the emotional consequences of repeated social stress in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1885–1900. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
48. Schellekens H, Dinan TG, Cryan JF. Ghrelin at the interface of obesity and reward. Vitam Horm. 2013;91:285–323. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
*49. Markus CR, Jonkman LM, Capello A, Leinders S, Husch F. Sucrose preload reduces snacking after mild mental stress in healthy participants as a function of 5-hydroxytryptamine transporter gene promoter polymorphism. Stress. 2015;18:149–159. The authors show that after a laboratory stressor, human subjects generally choose to eat sweet high-fat snacks more than savory high-fat snacks. However, this stress-induced preference for sweet fatty foods occurs primarily for people who are homozygous for a short-allele polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene. This work suggests that individual differences in stress-eating behavior may be related to genetic differences in serotonin neurotransmission. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
50. Capello AE, Markus CR. Differential influence of the 5-HTTLPR genotype, neuroticism and real-life acute stress exposure on appetite and energy intake. Appetite. 2014;77:83–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
51. Polivy J, Herman CP, McFarlane T. Effects of anxiety on eating: does palatability moderate distress-induced overeating in dieters? J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103:505–510. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
52. Gibson EL. Emotional influences on food choice: Sensory, physiological and psychological pathways. Physiol Behav. 2006;89:53–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
53. Macht M, Mueller J. Immediate effects of chocolate on experimentally induced mood states. Appetite. 2007;49:667–674. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
54. Tomiyama AJ, Dallman MF, Epel ES. Comfort food is comforting to those most stressed: evidence of the chronic stress response network in high stress women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:1513–1519. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
*55. Tryon MS, DeCant R, Laugero KD. Having your cake and eating it too: a habit of comfort food may link chronic social stress exposure and acute stress-induced cortisol hyporesponsiveness. Physiol Behav. 2013;114-115:32–37. This study suggests that women with both high levels of chronic stress and low cortisol responses to an acute lab stress are more prone to (1) stress-induced eating of sweet foods (chocolate cake), and (2) greater body fat. These associations support the idea that individuals that ‘self-medicate’ with high-sugar foods have lower HPA stress reactivity, and also support a potential mechanistic role for enlarged adipose depots in this process. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
**56. Tryon MS, Stanhope KL, Epel ES, Mason AE, Brown R, Medici V, Havel PJ, Laugero KD. Excessive Sugar Consumption May Be a Difficult Habit to Break: A View From the Brain and Body. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2015;100:2239–2247. This is an interventional study in women that consumed either sugar- or aspartame-sweetened beverages three times each day for two weeks (in addition to their normal diet). The authors found that women who drank sucrose, but not aspartame, had lower salivary cortisol responses to a laboratory stressor. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
57. Markus R, Panhuysen G, Tuiten A, Koppeschaar H. Effects of food on cortisol and mood in vulnerable subjects under controllable and uncontrollable stress. Physiol Behav. 2000;70:333–342. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
58. Ulrich-Lai YM, Christiansen AM, Ostrander MM, Jones AA, Jones KR, Choi DC, Krause EG, Evanson NK, Furay AR, Davis JF, et al. Pleasurable behaviors reduce stress via brain reward pathways. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:20529–20534. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
59. Strack AM, Akana SF, Horsley CJ, Dallman MF. A hypercaloric load induces thermogenesis but inhibits stress responses in the SNS and HPA system. Am J Physiol. 1997;272:R840–848. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
60. Suchecki D, Antunes J, Tufik S. Palatable solutions during paradoxical sleep deprivation: reduction of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity and lack of effect on energy imbalance. J Neuroendocrinol. 2003;15:815–821. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
61. Foster MT, Warne JP, Ginsberg AB, Horneman HF, Pecoraro NC, Akana SF, Dallman MF. Palatable foods, stress, and energy stores sculpt corticotropin-releasing factor, adrenocorticotropin, and corticosterone concentrations after restraint. Endocrinology. 2009;150:2325–2333. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
62. Kinzig KP, Hargrave SL, Honors MA. Binge-type eating attenuates corticosterone and hypophagic responses to restraint stress. Physiol Behav. 2008;95:108–113. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
63. Maniam J, Morris MJ. Palatable cafeteria diet ameliorates anxiety and depression-like symptoms following an adverse early environment. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35:717–728. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
64. Bell ME, Bhargava A, Soriano L, Laugero K, Akana SF, Dallman MF. Sucrose intake and corticosterone interact with cold to modulate ingestive behaviour, energy balance, autonomic outflow and neuroendocrine responses during chronic stress. J Neuroendocrinol. 2002;14:330–342. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
65. Laugero KD, Bell ME, Bhatnagar S, Soriano L, Dallman MF. Sucrose ingestion normalizes central expression of corticotropin-releasing-factor messenger ribonucleic acid and energy balance in adrenalectomized rats: a glucocorticoid-metabolic-brain axis? Endocrinology. 2001;142:2796–2804. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
66. Corona-Perez A, Diaz-Munoz M, Rodriguez IS, Cuevas E, Martinez-Gomez M, Castelan F, Rodriguez-Antolin J, Nicolas-Toledo L. High Sucrose Intake Ameliorates the Accumulation of Hepatic Triacylglycerol Promoted by Restraint Stress in Young Rats. Lipids. 2015;50:1103–1113. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
67. Dallman MF, Pecoraro NC, la Fleur SE. Chronic stress and comfort foods: self-medication and abdominal obesity. Brain Behav Immun. 2005;19:275–280. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
*68. de Kloet AD, Krause EG, Solomon MB, Flak JN, Scott KA, Kim DH, Myers B, Ulrich-Lai YM, Woods SC, Seeley RJ, et al. Adipocyte glucocorticoid receptors mediate fat-to-brain signaling. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;56:110–119. The authors show that transgenic mice with reduced glucocorticoid receptor (GR) expression in adipose tissue have increased indices of HPA axis tone. This is the first study to show that a GR-dependent, fat-derived signal can act in brain to reduce central HPA axis tone. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
*69. Ulrich-Lai YM, Christiansen AM, Wang X, Song S, Herman JP. Statistical modeling implicates neuroanatomical circuit mediating stress relief by ‘comfort’ food. Brain Struct Funct. 2015 in press. The authors apply a variety of statistical approaches, including Bayesian modeling, to measures of plasticity-related proteins in stress- and reward-regulatory brain regions following sucrose intake in rats. The modeling predicts the most-probable neural circuitry underling stress relief by limited sucrose intake. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
70. Egan AE, Ulrich-Lai YM. Activation of physiological stress responses by a natural reward: Novel vs. repeated sucrose intake. Physiol Behav. 2015;150:43–52. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
71. Christiansen AM, Dekloet AD, Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP. “Snacking” causes long term attenuation of HPA axis stress responses and enhancement of brain FosB/deltaFosB expression in rats. Physiol Behav. 2011;103:111–116. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
72. Schwabe L, Hoffken O, Tegenthoff M, Wolf OT. Preventing the stress-induced shift from goal-directed to habit action with a beta-adrenergic antagonist. J Neurosci. 2011;31:17317–17325. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Formats: