Logo of jgimedspringer.com This journal Toc Alerts Submit Online Open Choice
J Gen Intern Med. 2014 Jul; 29(7): 987–995.
Published online 2014 Feb 19. doi: 10.1007/s11606-014-2795-z
PMCID: PMC4061371
PMID: 24549521

An Investigation of Associations Between Clinicians’ Ethnic or Racial Bias and Hypertension Treatment, Medication Adherence and Blood Pressure Control

Associated Data

Supplementary Materials

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND

Few studies have directly investigated the association of clinicians’ implicit (unconscious) bias with health care disparities in clinical settings.

OBJECTIVE

To determine if clinicians’ implicit ethnic or racial bias is associated with processes and outcomes of treatment for hypertension among black and Latino patients, relative to white patients.

RESEARCH DESIGN AND PARTICIPANTS

Primary care clinicians completed Implicit Association Tests of ethnic and racial bias. Electronic medical records were queried for a stratified, random sample of the clinicians’ black, Latino and white patients to assess treatment intensification, adherence and control of hypertension. Multilevel random coefficient models assessed the associations between clinicians’ implicit biases and ethnic or racial differences in hypertension care and outcomes.

MAIN MEASURES

Standard measures of treatment intensification and medication adherence were calculated from pharmacy refills. Hypertension control was assessed by the percentage of time that patients met blood pressure goals recorded during primary care visits.

KEY RESULTS

One hundred and thirty-eight primary care clinicians and 4,794 patients with hypertension participated. Black patients received equivalent treatment intensification, but had lower medication adherence and worse hypertension control than white patients; Latino patients received equivalent treatment intensification and had similar hypertension control, but lower medication adherence than white patients. Differences in treatment intensification, medication adherence and hypertension control were unrelated to clinician implicit bias for black patients (P = 0.85, P = 0.06 and P = 0.31, respectively) and for Latino patients (P = 0.55, P = 0.40 and P = 0.79, respectively). An increase in clinician bias from average to strong was associated with a relative change of less than 5 % in all outcomes for black and Latino patients.

CONCLUSIONS

Implicit bias did not affect clinicians’ provision of care to their minority patients, nor did it affect the patients’ outcomes. The identification of health care contexts in which bias does not impact outcomes can assist both patients and clinicians in their efforts to build trust and partnership.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s11606-014-2795-z) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

KEY WORDS: hypertension, healthcare disparities, discrimination, implicit bias, race/ethnicity, quality

INTRODUCTION

Ethnic and racial health disparities persist in health care, with long-standing concerns that clinician bias contributes to the problem.15 The Institute of Medicine (IOM)3 has called for research to identify clinical situations in which bias may operate.

Bias against minority groups is a more complex phenomenon than is commonly appreciated. Bias can be overt (explicit bias) or hidden and unconscious (implicit bias).69 Implicit bias is often present even when explicit bias is absent.10,11 Recent studies1214 show that clinicians are no different from the broader population, having substantial implicit bias against minority groups, even as they report little explicit bias. Greater implicit bias among clinicians, in turn, has been linked to less patient-centered interactions with black patients.1517 Evidence on the effects of clinician implicit bias on medical decisions in hypothetical scenarios has been mixed.13,1820

The linkage between clinician implicit bias and more technical processes and outcomes of care has not been directly investigated. This study was conducted to help fill that gap. We focused on primary care because it is the cornerstone of the health care system in the United States. Americans in 2009 made over 1 billion office visits for health care, of which the majority for both whites and blacks were to primary care clinicians.21 The most common diagnosis in these visits was essential hypertension.22 Due to the importance of primary care, the prevalence of hypertension, and documented ethnic/racial disparities in hypertension control,23 we investigated the potential association between clinicians’ implicit biases and ethnic/racial differences in their patients’ intensification of treatment, medication adherence and hypertension control.

METHODS

Study Participants and Data Collection: Clinicians

Adult primary care clinicians in the Denver metropolitan area were recruited in a prior study on ethnic/racial attitudes.12 Implicit bias data collected from these clinicians were included in the present study for the two participating organizations that had electronic patient data. Denver Health is an integrated safety-net system that sees more than 100,000 unique patients in its community clinics each year. Kaiser Permanente Colorado is a closed panel, group model, not for-profit health maintenance organization (HMO) with approximately 480,000 members in the Denver metropolitan area. Institutional Review Boards for each institution approved the study protocol.

Implicit Association Test

Implicit ethnic and racial biases were measured with two Implicit Association Tests (IATs).2426 The IAT assesses the relative speed with which one can respond to a group and positive vs. negative words. Implicit bias against ethnic minorities compared with whites, for example, is shown if one is significantly faster when minority faces and negative words require the same response while white faces and good words require another response, compared with the reverse pairing. The larger this performance difference, the stronger the implicit bias (see https://implicit.harvard.edu). The IAT has been widely used and its psychometric properties and methodological strengths and limitations have been extensively reviewed.2630 Implicit attitudes, as measured by the IAT over multiple points in time, have been shown to have reasonable stability.26,28 The two IATs completed by the clinicians in this study were validated in previous research to measure implicit bias against blacks compared with whites and against Latinos compared with whites.12,25 Possible scores ranged from −2 to +2, with negative scores indicating bias against whites, positive scores indicating bias against blacks or Latinos, and 0 indicating no bias.

Study Participants and Data Collection: Patients

Patients were identified through primary care visit patterns. Eligible patients saw a participating clinician for a majority of primary care visits, at least three times during the observation period (June 2006 – May 2009). Patients who saw two clinicians exactly 50:50 (8 % of the sample) were assigned to the most recent clinician. A prior study15 with this patient population produced 93 % agreement between this algorithm and the patients’ own identification of their primary care clinician.

The patients’ records were screened to identify those with two or more diagnoses of hypertension (401.xx–404.xx).31 Patients were then eligible for inclusion if: (a) there was no comorbidity that would significantly alter hypertension treatment (e.g., end-stage renal disease or hospice care); (b) they had at least one blood pressure (BP) recorded in the last 18 months of the observation period; and (c) their recorded ethnicity/race was either black, Latino or white. Patients were excluded if they had opted out of research studies. Additional requirements were set for each outcome. Because participation in the study could alter clinicians’ subsequent behavior with patients, treatment intensification and medication adherence were examined during the 3 years (June 2006–May 2009) just prior to the clinicians’ participation. BP records were analyzed through November 2009.

Conceptual Model Guiding Patient Measures

Conceptually, clinicians’ implicit bias may alter minority patients’ treatment and health in two ways, as shown in Fig. 1.5,32,33 First, implicit bias may have a direct, negative effect on clinicians’ decisions about their patients (Path A). Second, it may indirectly affect care processes through reductions in the quality of clinical interactions (Path B). The combination of these two processes would result in reductions in treatment intensification and patients’ medication adherence, which in turn would reduce patients’ hypertension control.

Conceptual model of the influence that clinician implicit bias may have on treatment, adherence and control of hypertension. Variables that are assessed in this study appear in bold print. Path A indicates the potential for clinician bias to affect decisions about treatment intensification. Path B indicates the potential for clinician bias to affect patients’ trust and commitment to medication adherence. Many additional factors that affect treatment, adherence and hypertension control, including recursive processes, are not shown in this figure.

Treatment Intensification

Established procedures were followed to assess treatment intensification on the basis of pharmacy dispensing records.34,35 A baseline regimen was defined for each patient by the antihypertensive medication fills in a 90-day period, initiated by the first fill following the patient’s first visit with the clinician during the observation period.

Observed treatment intensification was defined as a dose increase of an existing antihypertensive medication or the addition of a new class of antihypertensive medication. An expected treatment intensification was defined as a visit in which a recorded BP was elevated above guidelines for hypertension control, contained in the Seventh Report of the Joint National Committee on Prevention, Detection, Evaluations, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure (JNC7).36 Using a standard scoring method,34,35,37,38 appropriate treatment intensification was calculated after the baseline period, by subtracting the number of expected intensifications from the number of observed intensifications, and then dividing this difference by the number of primary care visits during the observation period. This score had a range of −1 to 1, with −1 indicating no intensification at any visit with an elevated BP, 0 indicating intensification at every visit with an elevated BP, and 1 indicating intensification at every visit regardless of BP. Patients could only be included in this analysis if they had filled antihypertensive prescriptions during the baseline and follow-up periods, and if they had BP measures during the follow-up period.

Medication Adherence

Established procedures were followed to calculate medication adherence on the basis of pharmacy records.34,35 Medication adherence was calculated for each patient only after the patient’s first visit with the clinician during the observation period. At least two antihypertensive medication fills following that first visit were necessary for inclusion in the analysis.

A standard adherence score was calculated as the percent of days covered34,35,39,40 (0–100 %), based on the number of days of antihypertensive medication obtained divided by the number of days between the first and last refill. For patients receiving multiple antihypertensive medications, an average score was calculated.

Hypertension Control

All electronic BP records were obtained for outpatient primary care visits during the observation period.41,42 To ensure appropriate connection to the clinician, BP was analyzed only after the patient’s first visit with the clinician in that period. If more than one BP was recorded in a single day, the average for that day was used.

The primary measure of hypertension control was the percentage of time (0–100 %) that patients’ BP met JNC7 guidelines.36 Specifically, for each patient, each BP record in the observation period was characterized as meeting or not meeting JNC7 guidelines, and then this designation was given for the number of days until the next BP record. The total number of days designated as meeting guidelines was then divided by the total number of days between the first and last BP records, to control for the amount of time that was considered for that patient. Secondary measures of hypertension control included the percentage of time (0–100 %) the patients were kept out of Stage 2 hypertension36 and longitudinal analysis of all of the patients’ systolic BP (SBP) records across the observation period. We focused on SBP because uncontrolled SBP has a stronger association with important health outcomes than diastolic BP.43 An analysis with diastolic BP produced the same results (data not shown).

Comorbidities

ICD-9 codes were obtained for the observation period and the year prior. The comorbidity algorithms developed by Quan and colleagues44 were used to define relevant comorbidities: diabetes, congestive heart failure (CHF), coronary heart or artery disease (CHD), cerebrovascular disease (CVD), and peripheral vascular or arterial disease (PVD). Lab records for serum creatinine were also obtained to calculate eGFR for chronic kidney disease (CKD; eGFR <60 mL/min/1.73 m2), using the abbreviated Modification of Diet in Renal Disease study equation.45

Sociodemographics

Patients’ age, gender and ethnicity/race were obtained from administrative records. A prior study15 with these patients showed that the records were 90 % consistent with patients’ self-identified ethnicity/race.

Statistical Analysis

The primary effects of interest were ethnic or racial differences in treatment intensification, medication adherence and control of hypertension, and the associations between these differences and levels of clinician implicit bias. The data were analyzed using multilevel random coefficient modeling (mixed effects models), with patients modeled at level 1, clinicians at level 2 and clinics at level 3. This analysis provided estimation of the main effects at each level, as well as testing the effects of the interaction between patients’ ethnicity/race (level 1) and clinician implicit bias (level 2). The clinicians’ IAT scores for bias against blacks were used as predictors of outcomes for black patients; IAT scores for bias against Latinos were used as predictors of outcomes for Latino patients. Because the IAT measures relative bias (black vs. white or Latino vs. white), white patients always served as the ethnic/racial reference group.

All outcomes were analyzed as continuous variables. A common set of patient covariates was included in each analysis: age, gender and comorbidities (diabetes, CHF, CHD, CVD, PVD & CKD). Patients’ ethnicity/race, gender, age (<60 vs. 60+ yrs) and the presence of each comorbidity were analyzed as dichotomous. The longitudinal analysis of patients’ BP also included time (in years) as a parameter.

Sampling and Power Analysis

Based on the ethical principle of accessing only the patient records necessary to achieve our scientific aim, stratified random sampling was used. We randomly selected equal numbers of patients from the three ethnic/racial groups and approximately equal representation from age and gender groups for each primary care clinician, when sufficient patients were available. Initial sample size calculations were based on conservative estimates of the interclass correlations.

With our observed sample sizes and interclass correlations (e.g., 0.6 % unexplained variance among clinicians for percent time at JNC7 guidelines), we had 90 % power (alpha = 0.05, two-sided, using hierarchical model analysis) to detect associations between clinician implicit bias and ethnic/racial variation in outcomes that correspond to correlations as low as 0.12 and 0.09 for black and Latino patients, respectively. For example, for percent time at JNC7 guidelines, we had 90 % power to find a statistically significant effect as small as a 3.57 % difference for black patients (2.78 % for Latino patients), associated with a one standard deviation change in clinician implicit bias.

RESULTS

From the original sample of clinicians who completed measures of ethnic/racial bias,12 138 were eligible for this study because their organizations maintained electronic health records (66 % of the original sample; 52 % of primary care clinicians in the participating organizations). These clinicians were 54 % female, 83 % white, and 49 % had more than 10 years of clinical experience. Nearly 70 % of the clinicians showed some implicit bias against blacks or Latinos; 42 % of the sample had moderate-to-strong levels of bias against blacks and 51 % of the sample had moderate-to-strong levels of bias against Latinos.

Figure 2 shows the patient selection process for this study. Table 1 shows the characteristics of the final sample of 4,794 patients (982 black; 1,484 Latino; 2,328 white). Compared to the white patients, the minority patients were slightly younger, had more visits with their clinicians and had similar rates of comorbidities, with the exceptions of diabetes (higher among Latinos), and PVD and CKD (higher among Whites).

Flow of hypertension patients included in the study. Notes: *2 excluded for no BP recorded after first visit with clinician; **1,611 excluded: 505 had no anti-hypertension prescription fills at all, 228 had no fills following first visit with clinician, 597 had less than two BP, and 281 had no fills during the follow-up period; ***966 excluded: 505 had no anti-hypertension prescription fills at all, 228 had no fills following first visit with clinician, and 233 had only one fill.

Table 1

Clinical and Demographic Characteristics of Hypertension Patients by Ethnicity/Race

Characteristics Black
n = 982
Latino
n = 1,484
White
n = 2,328
Age M (SD)* 57 (12) 60 (13) 63 (13)
Female Gender (%) 574 (59 %) 878 (59 %) 1,330 (57 %)
Proportion of visits with clinician M (SD) 0.79 (0.17) 0.78 (0.17) 0.78 (0.17)
Number of visits with clinician M (SD)*** 9.9 (6.9) 10.2 (7.1) 9.3 (6.5)
Comorbidities
 Diabetes (%)*** 355 (36 %) 745 (50 %) 800 (34 %)
 CHF (%) 87 (9 %) 114 (8 %) 163 (7 %)
 CHD (%) 136 (14 %) 226 (15 %) 380 (16 %)
 CVD (%) 73 (7 %) 131 (9 %) 223 (10 %)
 PVD (%)*** 45 (5 %) 80 (5 %) 192 (8 %)
 CKD (%)*** 298 (30 %) 518 (35 %) 1,105 (48 %)

***Racial/ethnic groups differ significantly, p

Treatment Intensification

Average levels of treatment intensification did not differ significantly across groups (P = 0.31). As shown in Table 2, averages of −0.37 to −0.40 indicate that intensification occurred 37 %–40 % less frequently than would be expected by the number of elevated BP readings. Variation in treatment intensification was not associated with clinician implicit bias (P = 0.85 for black patients and P = 0.55 for Latino patients; see Table 3 for full parameter estimates). As shown in Table 4, a 1 SD increase in clinician bias (e.g., from average to strong bias) was associated with no change in treatment intensification for black patients and an increase of 0.01 in intensification for Latino patients.

Table 2

Adjusted Outcome Averages by Patients’ Ethnicity/Race

Outcome Black Latino White
Treatment Intensification −0.40 −0.37 −0.39
Percent Days Covered with Antihypertensive Medication 79.3 %*** 81.4 %*** 86.8 %
Percent Time at JNC7 Goal 43.5 %* 47.7 % 47.4 %
Percent Time out of Stage 2 Hypertension 87.0 %*** 90.7 % 91.5 %
Systolic BP (mm HG) 134.8*** 132.8 132.5
Diastolic BP (mm HG) 81.0*** 78.5 78.9
Number of Antihypertensive Medications
 Baseline 2.01*** 1.78 1.70
 Follow-up 2.53*** 2.24* 2.13

*p p 

NOTES: Treatment Intensification was the proportion of time that patients’ anti-hypertension medications were increased in dosage or class, given an elevated BP; a negative proportion indicates fewer intensifications than elevated BP. All outcomes were adjusted for patients’ age, gender and comorbidities (diabetes, CHF, CHD, CVD, PVD and CKD). Systolic and diastolic BP were also adjusted for time. The number of patients in each group differs across outcomes (see Fig. 2)

Table 3

Multilevel Model Estimates and Standard Errors for Processes and Outcomes Associated with Hypertension Treatment

Antihypertensive Treatment Intensification Percent Days Antihypertensive Meds Covered Percent Time BP at JNC7 Goal Percent Time out of Stage 2 Hypertension
Estimate SE Estimate SE Estimate SE Estimate SE
Covariance Parameters
 Clinic 0.002 0.001 7.3 4.0 28.3 11.1 4.8 2.5
 PCP 0.001 0.001 8.5 2.9 5.9 4.3 3.7 1.8
 Residual 0.127 0.003 339.1 7.9 896.3 18.7 323.0 6.7
Fixed-Effect Parameters
 Intercept −0.281 0.017 83.7 0.9 58.9 1.5 91.9 0.8
 Male 0.018 0.013 0.1 0.6 −1.5 0.9 −0.9 0.6
 60+ years −0.003 0.014 6.0*** 0.7 0.2 1.0 −0.4 0.6
 Diabetes −0.143*** 0.013 0.3 0.6 −14.3*** 0.9 0.8 0.6
 CHF 0.078** 0.025 −3.2** 1.2 2.6 1.8 −0.3 1.1
 CHD 0.057** 0.019 0.6 0.9 6.1*** 1.3 1.5 0.8
 CVD −0.005 0.022 −0.1 1.1 2.9 1.6 −0.5 1.0
 PVD 0.016 0.025 0.4 1.2 3.6* 1.8 −0.5 1.1
 CKD −0.173*** 0.014 −0.6 0.7 −17.7*** 1.0 −0.5 0.6
 Black −0.012 0.018 −7.5*** 0.9 −3.9** 1.2 −4.6*** 0.7
 Black:White IAT 0.007 0.030 −3.8* 1.7 −1.9 2.2 −1.2 1.4
 Race*IAT −0.011 0.059 5.3 2.9 −4.1 4.1 3.2 2.5
 Latino 0.016 0.015 −5.4*** 0.7 0.3 1.1 −0.9 0.6
 Latino:White IAT 0.014 0.024 1.3 1.4 2.3 1.8 −1.0 1.1
 Ethnicity*IAT 0.022 0.038 −1.5 1.8 −0.7 2.6 −0.2 1.6

Estimates different from zero, *p p p 

NOTE: Patient gender, age, comorbidities and race are dichotomous (0 or 1), and IAT scores are mean-centered. The intercept is the estimate for young, white females without comorbidities. Race*IAT is the interaction between patient race (black vs. white) and the Black:White IAT; Ethnicity*IAT is the interaction between patient ethnicity (Latino vs. white) and the Latino:White IAT

Table 4

Predicted Outcomes for Black and Latino Patients Associated with a 1 SD Increase in Clinician Implicit Bias

Outcome Black Patients Latino Patients
Black:White IAT Latino:White IAT
M Bias
(IAT = 0.30)
+1 SD Bias
(IAT = 0.59)
M Bias
(IAT = 0.32)
+1 SD Bias
(IAT = 0.70)
Treatment Intensification −0.40 −0.40 −0.37 −0.36
Days Covered with Antihypertensive Medication 79.3 % 79.7 % 81.4 % 81.3 %
Time at JNC7 BP Goal 43.5 % 41.8 % 47.7 % 48.3 %
Time out of Stage 2 Hypertension 87.0 % 87.6 % 90.7 % 90.3 %
Systolic BP (mm HG) 134.8 134.7 132.8 133.0

NOTES: Predictions are adjusted for patients’ age, gender and comorbidities (diabetes, CHF, CHD, CVD, PVD and CKD); white patients served as the reference group in each analysis. SBP is also adjusted for time. None of the predicted changes were statistically significant

Medication Adherence

Average levels of medication adherence were lower for black and Latino patients than white patients (P P = 0.06 for black patients and P = 0.40, for Latino patients; Table 3). A 1 SD increase in clinician bias was associated with 0.4 % increase in adherence for black patients and 0.1 % decrease in adherence for Latino patients.

Hypertension Control

The average percentage of time that patients’ BP met guidelines was lower for black patients (43.5 %, P P = 0.31 for black patients and P = 0.79 for Latino patients; Table 3). A 1 SD increase in clinician implicit bias was associated with 1.7 % (6 days/year) decrease for black patients and 0.6 % (2 days/year) increase for Latino patients.

Secondary measures of hypertension control produced equivalent results. Black patients spent less time than white patients out of Stage 2 hypertension (87.0 % vs. 91.5 %, P P P = 0.16) or SBP over time (P = 0.43; Table 2). Clinician implicit bias was not associated with time out of Stage 2 hypertension (P = 0.19 for black patients and P = 0.90 for Latino patients; Table 3), nor with SBP over time (P = 0.89 for black patients and P = 0.63 for Latino patients; full parameter estimates are available in online appendix).

DISCUSSION

This study found no evidence that the implicit biases of primary care clinicians were associated with less treatment intensification, worse adherence or worse BP control for black or Latino patients receiving care for hypertension. Our conceptual model suggests two routes through which clinician bias may produce ethnic/racial health disparities: A direct effect on clinical decision-making, and an indirect effect through communication and clinical interaction. A closer look at the evidence suggests that additional factors may need to be considered.

Research on the direct route has produced mixed results, and until now was entirely derived from surveys using hypothetical clinical vignettes. A widely cited report by Green and colleagues13 found that resident clinicians with greater implicit bias were less likely to recommend thrombolytic therapy for a hypothetical black patient with myocardial infarction, but this did not occur when the patient was described as white. On the other hand, a study18,19 on pediatric decision-making showed that some of the hypothetical decisions were associated with implicit bias, but others were not. Finally, a study20 with medical students failed to find any relation between hypothetical clinical decisions and the students’ implicit bias. The null results obtained in the current study suggest that the medical decisions of these experienced, primary care clinicians were not influenced by implicit bias, at least not to an extent that compromised the care processes and outcomes that were investigated with the patients included in this study.

Evidence on the second, communication-based route has been more consistent. Several studies1517 found that clinician implicit bias was associated with worse clinical interactions with black patients. In our own work with the same clinicians and patient population studied here,15 black patients reported less patient-centered treatment from clinicians who had higher levels of implicit bias. Communication issues are in and of themselves important, and other research suggests that lower quality communication is associated with less continuity of care, worse adherence and worse outcomes over time.4648 Research has yet to show, however, that the effects of bias on communication has these downstream effects. The null results obtained in the current study suggest that the bias-associated communication problems we previously observed15 did not translate into less intensive treatment, worse adherence or worse hypertension control for patients.

The conditions of our study identify some of the factors that may determine whether or not clinician bias contributes to ethnic/racial health disparities. Primary care has a number of features that are likely to mitigate the impact of bias. First is the opportunity in primary care for patients and clinicians to develop strong working relationships. The patients in our study received regular care from the same clinician for an average of nine visits across 3 years. Second, the processes and outcomes we examined were assessed over the course of years, with many opportunities for adjustment to address the patients’ medical needs in ways that avoided the influence of bias. Third, there are strong expectations for meeting hypertension control guidelines in both of the organizations included in this study, and there is general awareness of the problem of uncontrolled hypertension, particularly among blacks. Finally, there are checks and balances that often occur in primary care teams (e.g., clinicians, nurses and pharmacists), particularly in the integrated health care systems we studied. Implicit bias may be more likely to affect care delivered outside of established relationships, or without teams or systems support, or in decisions made under time pressure, with limited information or without the benefit of clear guidelines.33,49

This study has several limitations. The results are limited to primary care clinicians and their patients with diagnosed hypertension who received regular care. The study results are also limited to the two largest minority groups in this geographic area (blacks and Latinos); there were not enough patients in other groups to include them in the analysis. Clinician implicit bias may have greater effects on care processes and outcomes under different conditions than those studied here, as outlined previously. Pharmacy fills provide only a proxy for clinicians’ intentions for treatment intensification. Finally, the disparities in hypertension care between white, Black and Latino patients in these two delivery systems were small in magnitude; stronger effects of bias may be evident in settings with larger disparities in care.

CONCLUSIONS

The belief that clinician bias contributes to health care disparities is widespread, as reflected in the 2003 IOM report.3 In a series of studies, we have shown that implicit bias is prevalent among clinicians,12 and that it is reflected in the perceptions of black patients regarding their interpersonal treatment,15 but in the primary care settings considered in the present study, it does not appear to affect treatment intensification, medication adherence or hypertension control. Together, these findings highlight the need to better understand the conditions under which bias may and may not affect health care. Careful studies are needed in other clinical domains to fully explore the potential influence of clinician bias on care outcomes.

Electronic Supplementary Material

Online Appendix(50K, pdf)

(PDF 50 kb)

Acknowledgements

We thank the following individuals for assistance with the study: Holen Hirsh (Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder); Leslie Wright, Heather Tavel and Anju Gupta (Institute for Health Research, Kaiser Permanente Colorado); and Jonathan Mroch (Denver Health). This study was supported by grant HL088198 and Dr. Daugherty was supported by award HL103776, both from the National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute of NIH. The authors have no conflicts of interest in this research. Drs. Diane L. Fairclough, Michael Bronsert and Irene V. Blair had full access to all the data in this study.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they do not have a conflict of interest.

REFERENCES

1. Finucane TE, Carrese JA. Racial bias in presentation of cases. J Gen Intern Med. 1990;5:120–121. doi: 10.1007/BF02600511. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
2. Schulman KA, Berlin JA, Harless W, et al. The effect of race and sex on physicians’ recommendations for cardiac catheterization. N Engl J Med. 1999;340:618–626. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199902253400806. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
3. Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson AR, editors. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Healthcare. Institute of Medicine. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
4. Thomson G. Discrimination in health care. Ann Intern Med. 1997;126:910–912. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-126-11-199706010-00011. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
5. van Ryn M, Fu SS. Paved with good intentions: do public health and human service clinicians contribute to racial/ethnic disparities in health? Am J Public Health. 2003;93(s):248–255. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.93.2.248. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
6. Blair IV, Banaji MR. Automatic and controlled processes in stereotype priming. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;70:1142–1163. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1142. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
7. Devine PG. Stereotypes and prejudice: their automatic and controlled components. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989;56:5–18. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
8. Dovidio JF, Kawakami K, Gaertner SL. Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;82:62–68. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.62. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
9. Gawronski B, Bodenhausen GV. Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: an integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychol Bull. 2006;132:692–731. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
10. Nosek BA, Banaji MR, Greenwald AG. Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration website. Group Dyn. 2002;6:101–115. doi: 10.1037/1089-2699.6.1.101. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
11. Nosek BA, Smyth FL. A multitrait-multimethod validation of the Implicit Association Test: Implicit and explicit attitudes are related but distinct constructs. Exp Psychol. 2007;54:14–29. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.14. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
12. Blair IV, Havranek EP, Price DW, Hanratty R, Fairclough DL, Farley T, Hirsch HK, Steiner JF. An assessment of biases against Latinos and blacks among primary care clinicians and community members. Am J Public Health. 2013;103:92–98. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.300812. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
13. Green AR, Carney DR, Pallin DJ, Ngo LH, Raymond KL, Iezzoni LI, Banaji MR. Implicit bias among physicians and its prediction of thrombolysis decisions for black and white patients. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22:1231–1238. doi: 10.1007/s11606-007-0258-5. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
14. Sabin JA, Nosek BA, Greenwald AG, Rivara FP. Physicians’ implicit and explicit attitudes about race by MD race, ethnicity and gender. J Health Care Poor Underserved. 2009;20:896–913. doi: 10.1353/hpu.0.0185. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
15. Blair IV, Steiner JF, Fairclough DL, Hanratty R, Price DW, Hirsch HK, Wright LA, Bronsert M, Karimkhani E, Magid DJ, Havranek EP. Clinicians’ implicit ethnic/racial bias and perceptions of care among black and latino patients. Ann Fam Med. 2013;11:43–52. doi: 10.1370/afm.1442. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
16. Cooper LA, Roter DL, Carson KA, Beach MC, Sabin JA, Greenwald AG, Inui TS. The associations of clinicians’ implicit attitudes about race with medical visit communication and patient ratings of interpersonal care. Am J Public Health. 2012;102:979–987. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300558. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
17. Penner LA, Dovidio JF, West TV, Gaertner SL, Albrecht TL, Dailey RK, Markova T. Aversive racism and medical interactions with black patients: a field study. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2009;46:436–440. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.11.004. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
18. Sabin JA, Rivara FP, Greenwald AG. Physician implicit attitudes and stereotypes about race and quality of medical care. Med Care. 2008;46:678–685. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0b013e3181653d58. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
19. Sabin JA, Greenwald AG. The influence of implicit bias on treatment recommendations for 4 common pediatric conditions: pain, urinary tract infection, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and asthma. Am J Public Health. 2012;102:988–995. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300621. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
20. Haider AH, Sexton J, Sriram N, et al. Association of unconscious race and social class bias with vignette-based clinical assessments by medical students. JAMA. 2011;306:942–951. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
21. National Center for Health Statistics. Health, United States, 2011: With Special Feature on Socioeconomic Status and Health. Hyattsville, MD. 2012.
22. National Ambulatory Medical Care Survey: 2009 Summary Tables. http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/ahcd/namcs_summary/2009_namcs_web_tables.pdf (Accessed 1/20/14).
23. Gu Q, Burt VL, Dillon CF, Yoo S. Trends in antihypertensive medication use and blood pressure control among United States adults with hypertension. Circulation. 2012;126:2105–2114. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.112.096156. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
24. Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwarz JLK. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74:1464–1480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
25. Blair IV, Judd CM, Havranek EP, Steiner JF. Using community data to test the discriminant validity of ethnic/racial group IATs. J Psychol. 2010;218:36–43. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
26. Lane KA, Banaji MR, Nosek BA, Greenwald AG. Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: IV: What we know (so far) about the method. In: Wittenbrink B, Schwarz N, editors. Implicit Measures of Attitudes. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 59–102. [Google Scholar]
27. Nosek BA, Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. Understanding and using the implicit association test: II. Method variables and construct validity. Pers Soc Psychol B. 2005;31:166–180. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271418. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
28. Nosek BA, Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. The implicit association test at age 7: A methodological and conceptual review. In: Bargh JA, editor. Social Psychology and the Unconscious: The Automaticity of Higher Mental Processes. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2007. pp. 265–292. [Google Scholar]
29. Greenwald AG, Poehlman TA, Uhlmann E, Banaji MR. Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-Analysis of Predictive Validity. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2009;97:17–41. doi: 10.1037/a0015575. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
30. De Houwer J, Teige-Mocigemba S, Spruyt A, Moors A. Implicit measures: a normative analysis and review. Psychol Bull. 2009;135:347–368. doi: 10.1037/a0014211. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
31. Hanratty R, Estacio RO, Dickinson LM, Chandramouli V, Steiner JF, Havranek EP. Testing electronic algorithms to create disease registries in a safety net system. J Health Care Poor Underserved. 2008;19:452–465. doi: 10.1353/hpu.0.0027. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
32. Blair IV, Steiner JF, Havranek EP. Unconscious (implicit) bias and health disparities: where do we go from here? Perm J. 2011;15:71–78. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
33. Dovidio JF, Penner LA, Albrecht TL, Norton WE, Gaertner SL, Shelton JN. Disparities and distrust: the implications of psychological processes for understanding racial disparities in health and health care. Soc Sci Med. 2008;67:478–486. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.03.019. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
34. Daugherty SL, Powers D, Magid DJ, Masoudi FA, Margolis KL, O’Connor PJ, Schmittdiel JA, Ho PM. The association between medication adherence and treatment intensification with blood pressure control in resistant hypertension. Hypertension. 2012;60:303–309. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.112.192096. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
35. Maddox TM, Ross C, Tavel HM, Lyons EE, Tillquist M, Ho PM, Rumsfeld JS, Margolis KL, O’Connor PJ, Selby JV, Magid DJ. Blood pressure trajectories and associations with treatment intensification, medication adherence, and outcomes among newly diagnosed coronary artery disease patients. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes. 2010;3:347–357. doi: 10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.110.957308. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
36. National Heart Lung and Blood Institute. The seventh report of the Joint National Committee on prevention, detection, evaluation, and treatment of high blood pressure. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; 2004; NIH Publication No. 04–5230.
37. Okonofua EC, Simpson KN, Jesri A, Rehman SU, Durkalski VL, Egan BM. Therapeutic inertia is an impediment to achieving the Healthy People 2010 blood pressure control goals. Hypertension. 2006;47:345–351. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000200702.76436.4b. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
38. Rose AJ, Berlowitz DR, Manze M, Orner MB, Kressin NR. Comparing methods of measuring treatment intensification in hypertension care. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes. 2009;2:385–391. doi: 10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.108.838649. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
39. Steiner JF, Koepsell TD, Fihn SD, Inui TS. A general method of compliance assessment using centralized pharmacy records. Description and validation. Med Care. 1988;26:814–823. doi: 10.1097/00005650-198808000-00007. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
40. Steiner JF, Prochazka AV. The assessment of refill compliance using pharmacy records: methods, validity, and applications. J Clin Epidemiol. 1997;50:105–116. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(96)00268-5. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
41. Alexander M, Tekawa I, Hunkeler E, Fireman B, Rowell R, Selby JV, Massie BM, Cooper W. Evaluating hypertension control in a managed care setting. Arch Intern Med. 1999;159:2673–2677. doi: 10.1001/archinte.159.22.2673. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
42. Selby JV, Lee J, Swain BE, Tavel HM, Ho PM, Margolis KL, O’Connor PJ, Fine L, Schmittdiel JA, Magid DJ. Trends in time to confirmation and recognition of new-onset hypertension, 2002–2006. Hypertension. 2010;56:605–611. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.110.153528. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
43. Chobanian AV, Bakris GL, Black HR, Cushman WC, Green LA, Izzo JL, Jr, Jones DW, Materson BJ, Oparil S, Wright JT, Jr, Roccella EJ, the National High Blood Pressure Education Program Coordinating C Seventh report of the joint national committee on prevention, detection, evaluation, and treatment of high blood pressure. Hypertension. 2003;42:1206–1252. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000107251.49515.c2. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
44. Quan H, Sundararajan V, Halfon P, et al. Coding algorithms for defining comorbidities in ICD-9-CM and ICD-10 administrative data. Med Care. 2005;43:1130–1139. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000182534.19832.83. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
45. Levey AS, Coresh J, Greene T, Stevens LA, Zhang YL, Hendriksen S, Kusek JW, Van Lente F, Chronic Kidney Disease Epidemiology Collaboration Using standardized serum creatinine values in the modification of diet in renal disease study equation for estimating glomerular filtration rate. Ann Intern Med. 2006;145:247–254. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-145-4-200608150-00004. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
46. Safran DG, Taira DA, Rogers WH, Kosinski M, Ware JE, Tarlov AR. Linking primary care performance to outcomes of care. J Fam Pract. 1998;47(3):213–220. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
47. Schneider J, Kaplan SH, Greenfield S, Li W, Wilson IB. Better physician–patient relationships are associated with higher reported adherence to antiretroviral therapy in patients with HIV infection. J Gen Intern Med. 2004;19(11):1096–1103. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2004.30418.x. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
48. Wilson IB, Rogers WH, Chang H, Safran DG. Cost-related skipping of medications and other treatments among Medicare benefi ciaries between 1998 and 2000. Results of a national study. J Gen Intern Med. 2005;20(8):715–720. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2005.0128.x. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
49. Burgess DJ. Are providers more likely to contribute to healthcare disparities under high levels of cognitive load? How features of the healthcare setting may lead to biases in medical decision making. Med Dec Making. 2010;30:246–257. doi: 10.1177/0272989X09341751. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of General Internal Medicine are provided here courtesy of Society of General Internal Medicine

Formats: